Anyone who reads is bound to wonder, at least occasionally, about how those funny squiggles on a page magically turn into "Bare ruined choirs, where late the sweet birds sang" or "After a while I went out and left the hospital and walked back to the hotel in the rain." Where did this unlikely skill called reading come from? What happens in our brain when our eyes scan a line of type? Why do some of us, or some of our children, find it difficult to process the visual information held in words?
In Proust and the Squid, Maryanne Wolf, a professor at Tufts University and director of its Center for Reading and Language Research, offers explanations for all these questions, but with an emphasis that is "more biological and cognitive than cultural-historical." This means that Wolf focuses on the physiological character of the human brain, which holds at its disposal "three ingenious design principles: the capacity to make new connections among older structures; the capacity to form areas of exquisitely precise specialization for recognizing patterns in information, and the ability to learn to recruit and connect information from these areas automatically." These "design principles" provide the neuronal foundation of reading, and Wolf spends half her book explaining the evolution and minutiae of this "reading brain."
Nearly all this material makes for very hard slogging, even though Proust and the Squid is confidently described as the author's "first book for the general public." (The catchy but utterly uninformative title, by the way, refers to the novelist's impressionistic thoughts about childhood reading and a scientist's use of the squid brain for neurological research.) A work of popularization needs a light clear style, lots of anecdotes and some plot or story line that moves along at a good clip. At times, Wolf makes a stab at including some human-interest element or personal example, but all too soon she reverts to her normal prose, which is austere, technical and, finally, wearisome:
"In a pathbreaking meta-analysis of twenty-five imaging studies of different languages, cognitive scientists from the University of Pittsburgh found three great common regions used differentially across writing systems. In the first, the occipital-temporal area (which includes the hypothesized locus of 'neuronal recycling' for literacy), we become proficient visual specialists in whatever script we read. In the second, the frontal region around Broca's area, we become specialists in two different ways -- for phonemes in words and for their meanings. In the third, the multifunction region spanning the upper temporal lobes and the lower, adjacent parietal lobes, we recruit additional areas that help to process multiple elements of sounds and meanings, which are particularly important for alphabetic and syllabary systems."
Out of context such prose sounds perfectly dreadful -- and in context sadly characteristic of the writing in professional journals, no matter what the field. In fact, everything Wolf says makes sense, the specialized terms she uses have been previously defined, and there are line illustrations on a facing page. Nonetheless, such technical onslaughts are extremely tiring to read, and Wolf seldom lets up on the information-rich barrage for very long. At different points she does quote passages from Proust and George Eliot, but even these two great novelists are hardly what you'd call sprightly, and they merely add their own specific gravity to already forbidding pages.
In the second half of the book, Wolf examines the reading difficulties generally subsumed under the term dyslexia. We learn that one of her sons suffers from this disability, that there are various forms and theories about its origin and character, that it can sometimes result in a special talent for fields that emphasize pattern and spatial creativity (such as art, design and engineering) and that "programs which systematically and explicitly teach young readers phoneme awareness and grapheme-phoneme correspondence are far more successful in dealing with reading disabilities than other programs." As this last sentence makes evident, no relief awaits the once-eager reader who by this point has begun to wonder if he could be suffering from a sudden case of adult-onset dyslexia.
Despite Wolf's failure to write a truly popular book, she clearly does know her stuff, and those professionally involved with the teaching of reading might be more patient than I. In particular, she addresses the special needs of children raised in cultures where standard English isn't the dominant language, and she speculates, with real concern, about the impact of computer culture on the "reading brain." Dyslexia has taught her that humans were never genetically designed to read, and this peculiar technique of sustained mental attention could be reduced, reconfigured or even lost in the rising digital age:
"Will unguided information lead to an illusion of knowledge, and thus curtail the more difficult, time-consuming, critical thought processes that lead to knowledge itself? Will the split-second immediacy of information gained from a search engine and the sheer volume of what is available derail the slower, more deliberative processes that deepen our understanding of complex concepts, of another's inner thought processes, and of our own consciousness?"
Wolf never fully answers these questions, though they strike me as the basis for a much needed book. Still, like any parent with a child transfixed by flashing screens, she is troubled by what she observes. She urges that we "teach our children to be 'bitextual' " or 'multitextual,' able to read and analyze texts flexibly in different ways" so that our sons and daughters don't end up as mere "decoders of information," distracted from the "deeper development of their intellectual potential." Early on in Proust and the Squid, she had noted that infants and toddlers who aren't told stories by their caregivers, who aren't read to from a very early age, nearly always fail to learn to read well themselves. By implication, it may already be too late for many young people: They will never be able to read with the same thoughtfulness and comprehension as their parents. Think about that.
《创意之钥:Android手机交互应用开发》以android手机交互开发为平台,定位于创意从产生到实际应用的过程,将书中内容分为创意的真
《ROS机器人开发:实用案例分析(原书第2版)》内容简介:本书不仅涵盖ROS的基础知识,还循序渐进地讲解了ROS机器人的高级应用特性
《河南社会治理发展报告(2017)》内容简介:本书从社会安全、矛盾化解、社会组织、公共服务、社会公平五个维度,对河南省社会治理
《网众传播 (数字媒介变革书系)》内容简介:“网众传播”指的是由“网众”发起和参与,由“社会性媒体”中介的传播模式、现象与行
《名院名医孕产育儿全程指导方案:40周幸福怀孕》内容简介:在40周的孕期生活中,孕妈妈的身体会发生怎样的变化?胎宝宝会按照怎样
《大数据技术入门(第2版)》内容简介:目前国内大数据市场继续保持高速的发展态势,作者在与地方政府、证券金融公司的项目合作中发
论文排版实用教程-Word与LaTeX 本书特色 本书按照论文写作编排的顺序,先后以word、latex两种软件为例分别介绍论文的编排方法。第1章主要介绍有关论...
《OrangeS:一个操作系统的实现》从只有二十行的引导扇区代码出发,一步一步地向读者呈现一个操作系统框架的完成过程。书中不仅关
《暗黑家族》内容简介:怪死的姐姐的钱包里有一张纸条—— “这个家里住着恶魔。”原本幸福的家庭因姐姐的离去逐渐走向崩坏,潜藏于
《世界航母大百科》内容简介:本书以时间为主线,对绝大部分航空母舰逐一进行了介绍,每型配以简明扼要的文字说明、线图、历史照片
《合为一家》内容简介:十六国北魏时期是中国历史上民族融合的重要时期,也是多元文化激荡的时期,各民族之间的互动非常频繁,混居
《三维CAD/CAM》内容简介:结合机械类专业的实际,以够用为度,主要讲述设计模块和加工模块。第一部分为设计模块(即CAD模块),主
本书是《DataStructuresandAlgorithmAnalysisinC》一书第2版的简体中译本。原书曾被评为20世纪顶尖的30部计算机著作之一,作者...
《白鹭在冰面上站着》内容简介:无论被叫作香格里拉,还是勐巴拉西,云南都是“人间天堂”的意思。在雷平阳眼里,云南是他的文学诞
Computationalcomplexityisoneofthemostbeautifulfieldsofmodernmathematics,anditisi...
老兵新传Visual Basic核心编程及通用模块开发 本书特色 编程“高手”的功力体现在对语言的彻底掌握和丰富的实践经验书稿内容由作者原创编写,直接面向实际应...
《知行合一:悦读《传习录》》内容简介:《传习录》是明朝著名的哲学家、政治家、教育家和军事家王阳明的一部语录体哲学著作,内容
网页中的图形设计是在线内容用户体验的关键,尤其是现在的用户会使用多种设备访问网页,如智能手机、平板电脑、笔记本电脑、桌面
《守正出奇》内容简介:对数字化传播的本质和特征的探讨,是《守正出奇:传统美妆品牌的数字化传播》的研究定位和目标;以美妆品牌
数学之美-第二版 本书特色 几年前,“数学之美”系列文章原刊载于谷歌黑板报,获得上百万次点击,得到读者高度评价。 正式出版前,吴军博士几乎把所有文章都重写了一遍...