Anyone who reads is bound to wonder, at least occasionally, about how those funny squiggles on a page magically turn into "Bare ruined choirs, where late the sweet birds sang" or "After a while I went out and left the hospital and walked back to the hotel in the rain." Where did this unlikely skill called reading come from? What happens in our brain when our eyes scan a line of type? Why do some of us, or some of our children, find it difficult to process the visual information held in words?
In Proust and the Squid, Maryanne Wolf, a professor at Tufts University and director of its Center for Reading and Language Research, offers explanations for all these questions, but with an emphasis that is "more biological and cognitive than cultural-historical." This means that Wolf focuses on the physiological character of the human brain, which holds at its disposal "three ingenious design principles: the capacity to make new connections among older structures; the capacity to form areas of exquisitely precise specialization for recognizing patterns in information, and the ability to learn to recruit and connect information from these areas automatically." These "design principles" provide the neuronal foundation of reading, and Wolf spends half her book explaining the evolution and minutiae of this "reading brain."
Nearly all this material makes for very hard slogging, even though Proust and the Squid is confidently described as the author's "first book for the general public." (The catchy but utterly uninformative title, by the way, refers to the novelist's impressionistic thoughts about childhood reading and a scientist's use of the squid brain for neurological research.) A work of popularization needs a light clear style, lots of anecdotes and some plot or story line that moves along at a good clip. At times, Wolf makes a stab at including some human-interest element or personal example, but all too soon she reverts to her normal prose, which is austere, technical and, finally, wearisome:
"In a pathbreaking meta-analysis of twenty-five imaging studies of different languages, cognitive scientists from the University of Pittsburgh found three great common regions used differentially across writing systems. In the first, the occipital-temporal area (which includes the hypothesized locus of 'neuronal recycling' for literacy), we become proficient visual specialists in whatever script we read. In the second, the frontal region around Broca's area, we become specialists in two different ways -- for phonemes in words and for their meanings. In the third, the multifunction region spanning the upper temporal lobes and the lower, adjacent parietal lobes, we recruit additional areas that help to process multiple elements of sounds and meanings, which are particularly important for alphabetic and syllabary systems."
Out of context such prose sounds perfectly dreadful -- and in context sadly characteristic of the writing in professional journals, no matter what the field. In fact, everything Wolf says makes sense, the specialized terms she uses have been previously defined, and there are line illustrations on a facing page. Nonetheless, such technical onslaughts are extremely tiring to read, and Wolf seldom lets up on the information-rich barrage for very long. At different points she does quote passages from Proust and George Eliot, but even these two great novelists are hardly what you'd call sprightly, and they merely add their own specific gravity to already forbidding pages.
In the second half of the book, Wolf examines the reading difficulties generally subsumed under the term dyslexia. We learn that one of her sons suffers from this disability, that there are various forms and theories about its origin and character, that it can sometimes result in a special talent for fields that emphasize pattern and spatial creativity (such as art, design and engineering) and that "programs which systematically and explicitly teach young readers phoneme awareness and grapheme-phoneme correspondence are far more successful in dealing with reading disabilities than other programs." As this last sentence makes evident, no relief awaits the once-eager reader who by this point has begun to wonder if he could be suffering from a sudden case of adult-onset dyslexia.
Despite Wolf's failure to write a truly popular book, she clearly does know her stuff, and those professionally involved with the teaching of reading might be more patient than I. In particular, she addresses the special needs of children raised in cultures where standard English isn't the dominant language, and she speculates, with real concern, about the impact of computer culture on the "reading brain." Dyslexia has taught her that humans were never genetically designed to read, and this peculiar technique of sustained mental attention could be reduced, reconfigured or even lost in the rising digital age:
"Will unguided information lead to an illusion of knowledge, and thus curtail the more difficult, time-consuming, critical thought processes that lead to knowledge itself? Will the split-second immediacy of information gained from a search engine and the sheer volume of what is available derail the slower, more deliberative processes that deepen our understanding of complex concepts, of another's inner thought processes, and of our own consciousness?"
Wolf never fully answers these questions, though they strike me as the basis for a much needed book. Still, like any parent with a child transfixed by flashing screens, she is troubled by what she observes. She urges that we "teach our children to be 'bitextual' " or 'multitextual,' able to read and analyze texts flexibly in different ways" so that our sons and daughters don't end up as mere "decoders of information," distracted from the "deeper development of their intellectual potential." Early on in Proust and the Squid, she had noted that infants and toddlers who aren't told stories by their caregivers, who aren't read to from a very early age, nearly always fail to learn to read well themselves. By implication, it may already be too late for many young people: They will never be able to read with the same thoughtfulness and comprehension as their parents. Think about that.
Thelastdecadehaswitnessedaproliferationofartistswhoseprimarymediumissoftware.Alg...
《中国法制史》内容简介:中国法制史是研究中国法制发生、发展、演变及其规律的学科。它是中国制度史的一部分,是法学体系中的独立
《盛开·90后新概念·花样年华书系·樱花纪》内容简介:本书系新概念作文获奖者作品精华,约32万字。本书中这些作品共分五辑,每个
《Go微服务实战》内容简介:当今世界,软件的规模越来越大、功能越来越复杂,研发团队的规模也变得越来越大,运维人员和研发人员之
《周作人散文》内容简介:《中华散文珍藏版:周作人散文》是“中华散文珍藏版”系列丛书之一种,精选了周作人经典散文90篇,如:《
《元宇宙基石:Web3.0与分布式存储》内容简介:本书Web3.0深刻地变革了数据存储的方式,使个人不再依赖数据存储费高昂的企业来存储
物流仓储配送系统设计技巧450问 本书特色 本书通过大量的公式、图形、表格、实例,系统地介绍了物流配送中心、自动化仓库、堆垛机、输送机、储存机械、高密度储存、a...
《一个APP的诞生》内容简介:在移动互联网高度发达的今天,一个个APP,成为我们通向网络世界的窗口。它的诞生流程,令不少对互联网
计算机基础及Photoshop应用-全国计算机等级考试一级教程-(2015年版)-(含光盘) 本书特色 本书是根据教育部考试中心颁布的《全国计算机等级考试一级p...
《企业应用架构模式经典重读》内容简介:本书作者是当今面向对象软件开发的权威,他在一组专家级合作者的帮助下,将40多种经常出现
罗刚君编著的《ExcelVBA程序开发自学宝典(第2版)》是VBA入门的经典教材,对VBA的基础理论、语法规则、代码优化、编写思路、开发
《错觉》内容简介:在人工智能异常火热的今天,很多人认为我们生活在一个不可思议的历史时期,人工智能和大数据可能比工业革命更能
《数字解读中国》内容简介:改革开放以来,特别是党的十八大以来,中国经济建设和社会发展取得了举世瞩目的辉煌成就,中国稳居世界
BrandsAtoZ:是一套讲述富于独立性和创造性的品牌故事的丛书.本书详细探寻了每一个被选择品牌的历史,反映了其公司文化,并展示了其
FloTHERM软件基础与应用实例 本书特色 李波编写的《flotherm软件基础与应用实例》分为软件基础入门与应用实例两大部分,共12章:第1~9章为软件基础...
《新媒体写作平台策划与运营》内容简介:本书共分7章。第1章重点介绍了新媒体与自媒体的区别与联系、主流自媒体写作平台及变现途径
《高质量零售》内容简介:在技术变革和需求变革共同驱动下,新零售对零售业进行了全要素、多维度、系统化的创新与变革,实现了交易
全套上、下两册,浓缩2005年电脑报精华文章;附录包含70余篇简明IT应用指南,涵盖软件、硬件、数码、网络四大领域,配赠权威实用
InDesign CC实战从入门到精通-(全彩版) 本书特色 InDesign是当今流行的专业排版软件,被广泛应用于平面设计、印刷出版、数字媒体等诸多领域。本书...
LTE语音业务及VOLTE技术详解 本书特色 本书讲解了各种可能的lte语音业务技术解决方案,同时也介绍了lte**演进技术,即所谓的5g可能采用的先进技术和演...